Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Accepted Manuscripts
    • Article Preview
    • Past Issue Archive
    • AJNR Case Collection
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
  • Special Collections
    • Spinal CSF Leak Articles (Jan 2020-June 2024)
    • 2024 AJNR Journal Awards
    • Most Impactful AJNR Articles
  • Multimedia
    • AJNR Podcast
    • AJNR Scantastics
    • Video Articles
  • For Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Policies
    • Fast publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
    • Graphical Abstract Preparation
    • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
    • Imaging Protocol Submission
    • Submit a Case for the Case Collection
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editorial Board
  • More
    • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Advertisers
    • ASNR Home
  • Other Publications
    • ajnr

User menu

  • Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
American Journal of Neuroradiology
American Journal of Neuroradiology

American Journal of Neuroradiology

ASHNR American Society of Functional Neuroradiology ASHNR American Society of Pediatric Neuroradiology ASSR
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Accepted Manuscripts
    • Article Preview
    • Past Issue Archive
    • AJNR Case Collection
    • Case of the Week Archive
    • Classic Case Archive
    • Case of the Month Archive
  • Special Collections
    • Spinal CSF Leak Articles (Jan 2020-June 2024)
    • 2024 AJNR Journal Awards
    • Most Impactful AJNR Articles
  • Multimedia
    • AJNR Podcast
    • AJNR Scantastics
    • Video Articles
  • For Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Author Policies
    • Fast publishing of Accepted Manuscripts
    • Graphical Abstract Preparation
    • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
    • Imaging Protocol Submission
    • Submit a Case for the Case Collection
  • About Us
    • About AJNR
    • Editorial Board
  • More
    • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
    • Subscribers
    • Permissions
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Advertisers
    • ASNR Home
  • Follow AJNR on Twitter
  • Visit AJNR on Facebook
  • Follow AJNR on Instagram
  • Join AJNR on LinkedIn
  • RSS Feeds

Welcome to the new AJNR, Updated Hall of Fame, and more. Read the full announcements.


AJNR is seeking candidates for the position of Associate Section Editor, AJNR Case Collection. Read the full announcement.

 

Research ArticleFunctional
Open Access

Decreased Left Posterior Insular Activity during Auditory Language in Autism

J.S. Anderson, N. Lange, A. Froehlich, M.B. DuBray, T.J. Druzgal, M.P. Froimowitz, A.L. Alexander, E.D. Bigler and J.E. Lainhart
American Journal of Neuroradiology January 2010, 31 (1) 131-139; DOI: https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A1789
J.S. Anderson
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
N. Lange
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
A. Froehlich
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
M.B. DuBray
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
T.J. Druzgal
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
M.P. Froimowitz
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
A.L. Alexander
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
E.D. Bigler
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
J.E. Lainhart
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Responses
  • References
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Individuals with autism spectrum disorders often exhibit atypical language patterns, including delay of speech onset, literal speech interpretation, and poor recognition of social and emotional cues in speech. We acquired functional MR images during an auditory language task to evaluate systematic differences in language-network activation between control and high-functioning autistic populations.

MATERIALS AND METHODS: Forty-one right-handed male subjects (26 high-functioning autistic subjects, 15 controls) were studied by using an auditory phrase-recognition task, and areas of differential activation between groups were identified. Hand preference, verbal intelligence quotient (IQ), age, and language-function testing were included as covariables in the analysis.

RESULTS: Control and autistic subjects showed similar language-activation networks, with 2 notable differences. Control subjects showed significantly increased activation in the left posterior insula compared with autistic subjects (P < .05, false discovery rate), and autistic subjects showed increased bilaterality of receptive language compared with control subjects. Higher receptive-language scores on standardized testing were associated with greater activation of the posterior aspect of the left Wernicke area. A higher verbal IQ was associated with greater activation of the bilateral Broca area and involvement of the prefrontal cortex and lateral premotor cortex.

CONCLUSIONS: Control subjects showed greater activation of the posterior insula during receptive language, which may correlate with impaired emotive processing of language in autism. Subjects with autism showed greater bilateral activation of receptive-language areas, which was out of proportion to the differences in hand preference in autism and control populations.

Language disturbances are among the most pronounced and clinically significant features and strongest predictors of outcome in autism.1,2 Auditory and language disturbances in autism include delayed onset of speech,3 widely varying impairments in comprehension and spoken language, stereotypical or idiosyncratic speech patterns,4 and hypersensitivity to distracting or unexpected auditory stimuli.5 Though much less studied, language impairment in autism may also involve reading ability.6 Qualitatively similar though milder delays in speech, spoken language, and reading are also found in family members of children with autism.7 The neural basis of language-related impairments in autism is critical to understanding brain mechanisms driving the clinical impairments, developing interventions to improve the function and prognosis of affected individuals, and identifying genes and other risk factors involved.

There is converging evidence from multiple modalities that language in autistic subjects shows atypical hemispheric lateralization in the brain. It has long been established that the metrics of hand preference show decreased right-hand dominance in the autistic populations.8-10 Dichotic listening tasks show reduced right-ear advantage for speech processing in autism.11 Single-photon emission tomography and positron-emission tomography imaging show decreased relative cerebral blood flow lateralization in the language-related cortex for subjects with autism.12,13 An electroencephalography examination found reduced left lateralization of temporal lobe μ rhythms in an autistic population.14 Anomalous age-related changes in lateralization of language areas during later childhood were found for autistic subjects in a magnetoencephalography study.15 Although abnormal volumetric asymmetry of frontal but not superior temporal language areas has been consistently found in autism,16-18 functional asymmetry of the superior temporal gyrus has been observed repeatedly.15,19-26

Lateralization of language has been examined directly by using functional MR imaging (fMRI). In an expressive language letter fluency task, 14 high-functioning adolescent and adult males with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) were found to have less left-lateralized activation in frontal language areas than 14 controls.22 A separate study by using a semantically based response naming task demonstrated a larger difference in percentage signal-intensity change between the Broca area and its right homolog in 12 controls compared with 12 high-functioning males with ASD.27

We investigated receptive-language processing by using an auditory language task, with a paradigm that examined phrase and sentence levels of language processing to determine whether differences in activation outside core receptive and expressive language regions were seen between autism and typically developing groups.

Materials and Methods

Subject Characteristics

Twenty-six high-functioning males with autism were compared with 15 male healthy volunteer subjects, group-matched by age. Table 1 compares group demographics of age, handedness, receptive-language function, verbal intelligence quotient (IQ), and performance IQ of the autism and control populations. There was a slight trend toward decreased right-handedness in the autism group, which was not statistically significant in our sample. One autism participant was left-handed, and 2 controls were ambidextrous. The participants had no history of hearing problems, and all had English as their first language. All controls had normal language function. As expected, language function was impaired in the autism participants as a group. Verbal and performance IQ scores showed small but significant decreases in the autism group. Experimental procedures were approved by the University of Utah institutional review board. Informed consent was obtained for all subjects.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1:

Characterization of control and autism populations

Diagnosis and Exclusion Criteria

Diagnosis of autism was established by the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised,28 the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G),29 the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV,3 and the World Health Organization International Classification of Diseases–10 criteria, under the direction of a board-certified child psychiatrist. Participants were excluded if medical causes of autism were identified by participant history, fragile-X gene testing, karyotype, and observation.

Control participants underwent tests of IQ and language function and standardized psychiatric assessments30 and were assessed with the ADOS-G29 to confirm typical development. Controls with any history of developmental, learning, cognitive, neurologic, or neuropsychiatric conditions were excluded.

Assessments

Handedness.

The Edinburgh Handedness Inventory,31 a standardized assessment of hand preference, was performed for each subject. This inventory consists of a numeric score between −100 and 100, in which −100 represents strong left-handedness and 100 represents strong right-handedness.

IQ.

Verbal IQ (vIQ) and performance IQ (pIQ) were measured with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS III) or the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI).

Language.

The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 3rd ed (CELF-3),32 was used to assess language skills. It is a comprehensive and nationally normalized clinical assessment tool that provides a quantitative measure of language level. The CELF-3 includes subtests that measure grammar, syntax, semantics, and working memory for language and provides an overall assessment of higher order receptive and expressive language and a total language level. We used the “Receptive” subtest score as a covariable in our analysis because our fMRI task was primarily designed to measure receptive-language function.

fMRI Acquisition

Images were acquired on a Magnetom Trio (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) scanner. All fMRI subjects were fitted with MR imaging–compatible lenses to allow comfortable reading of the 8-point text within the scanner. Subject alertness was monitored throughout the examination by real-time eye tracking by using an infrared camera mounted on a 12-channel head coil (Siemens).

The scanning protocol consisted of an initial 1-mm isotropic magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition of gradient echo (MPRAGE) acquisition for an anatomic template. Blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) echo-planar images (TR = 2.0 seconds, TE = 28 ms, generalized autocalibrating partially parallel acquisition with acceleration factor = 2, 40 sections at 3-mm section thickness) were obtained during the auditory language task described below. Prospective motion correction was performed during BOLD imaging with a prospective acquisition-correction technique sequence.

We chose phrase- and sentence-level tasks rather than single-word tasks because the former are known to produce less variable and more sensitive activation maps compared with lexical-level tasks.33,34 The auditory language task consisted of a block-design alternating 20 seconds of auditory stimuli with 20 seconds of no stimuli. Auditory stimuli were delivered to sound-blocking pneumatic headphones (Silent Scan SS-3100; Avotec, Stuart, Florida) and consisted of phrases that described a common word. Examples of phrases include, “Jewelry we wear around our neck,” “The funny guys at the circus,” and “Water falling from the sky.” Subjects were instructed to think of a word that each phrase describes. Six phrases were presented during each block, with a 1-second pause between phrases for subjects to think of an appropriate word. During epochs with no stimuli, subjects were instructed to rest but keep their eyes open. A single run of the task, of 4-minute duration, was performed in all subjects.

The visual language task consisted of a similar 20-second block design. Visual stimuli were displayed via an LCD projector onto a screen in the bore of the scanner and were viewed by a mirror mounted on the top of the 12-channel head coil. Stimuli consisted of sentences with a blank at the end. Subjects were instructed to think of an appropriate word to complete the sentence. Sample sentences include, “She put the dishes in the _____,” and “He took a shortcut to go ____.” Eight sentences were presented per 20-second block. During epochs without sentences, subjects were instructed to fixate on a high-contrast mark in the center of the screen. A single run of the task, of 4-minute duration, was performed in 14 autism and 9 control subjects.

fMRI Postprocessing and Statistical Analysis

Off-line postprocessing was performed in Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, Massachusetts) by using SPM8b software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). A field map sequence was acquired for each subject for distortion correction, and all images were motion-corrected by using a realign and unwarp procedure. No difference in head motion was seen between autism and control groups during retrospective motion correction. Using maximal detected motion in x-, y-, and z-directions by a retrospective motion-correction algorithm, we computed the square root (x2 + y2 +z2) for each subject as an index of head motion. For the autism group, this index measured 0.8 ± 0.32 mm (range, 0.32–1.3 mm). For the control group, maximal head motion measured 0.71 ± 0.31 mm (range, 0.24–1.2 mm). Findings were not significantly different by using a 2-tailed t test (P =.35). BOLD images were coregistered to MPRAGE anatomic image sequences for each subject. All images were normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template brain (T1.nii in SPM8b), and smoothing with an 8-mm kernel was performed on all images.

Activation maps were generated for each subject by using a general linear model to obtain t-contrast images. No minimal cluster size was specified in any of the analyses. Second-level random-effects analysis was then performed for autism and control subjects on t-contrast images for each subject. Age, handedness, receptive-language ability (CELF-3), and vIQ were included as covariables in the second-level analysis. Activation maps were obtained separately for the control group and for the autism group, with each thresholded at an acceptable false discovery rate (FDR) of P < .05 (Fig 1).

Fig 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 1.

Group-level activation maps for an auditory phrase-recognition task for 15 control subjects (A) and for 26 high-functioning autistic subjects (B). Results for each group represent P < .05, false discovery rate (FDR), and color bars represent values for t-scores. Arrows show left posterior insular, right lateral premotor, and right Wernicke homolog areas where differences in activity are observed.

The visual sentence-completion task was analyzed by using a similar procedure, and auditory > visual contrast across all subjects was thresholded at an acceptable FDR, P < .05, to identify brain regions active preferentially during the auditory task, which is shown for typically developing controls in Fig 2(blue). This auditory > visual contrast yielded activation maps of bilateral superior temporal gyri in expected locations of the primary auditory cortex. To identify receptive-language clusters in each subject, this auditory > visual contrast was used as a mask to exclude voxels in the primary auditory cortex from the analysis. Maximal clusters were selected bilaterally from activated voxels posterior to the primary auditory cortex in the superior temporal, middle temporal, and supramarginal and angular gyri regions by using P < .05, FDR, as the threshold for each subject. The number of activated voxels in the left and right receptive-language clusters was used to calculate the fMRI laterality index by using the formula (left − right)/(left + right) to obtain a measurement between −1 (strongly right dominant) and 1 (strongly left dominant).

Fig 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 2.

Group-level activation maps for 15 control subjects for auditory > visual tasks (blue) and auditory tasks (red) after masking the auditory > visual tasks. Both results show P < .05, FDR.

Results

Magnitude of Activation during the Auditory Task

Activation maps for control and autism populations during the auditory language task are shown in Fig 1. All subjects showed activated clusters in expected language regions (Wernicke area, Broca area, lateral premotor cortex, supplementary motor area, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) when thresholded at an acceptable FDR, P < .05. Population activation maps showed remarkable similarity in the spatial distribution of activation, with similar t-scores and spatial coordinates of local maxima in the core language regions (Table 2). A few notable differences were seen. There was a focus of activation in the left posterior insula much greater than that in the control map (Fig 1, white arrow), and activation in the right Wernicke homolog extended farther posteriorly in the autism map than in the control map (Fig 1, black arrows). A trend toward increased activation in the right lateral premotor cortex in the autism map (Fig 1, black arrow) was not statistically significant in our sample. We detected no significant difference in the magnitude of auditory stimulus responses in the primary auditory cortical regions between groups. Cerebellar activation was also noted for most subjects, but the infratentorial brain was not consistently included in the FOV for all subjects and does not appear on activation maps.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2:

Activation of core auditory language regions

Differences in Auditory and Visual Task Activation

The visual task showed a similar spatial distribution of language activation, with additional posterior parietal, lateral temporo-occipital, and visual cortical areas. The difference between auditory and visual task activation, thresholded at an FDR of P < .05, is shown in blue in Fig 2, rendered on the MNI template brain image, with the remaining auditory task activation from all subjects shown in red. The auditory task showed significant increases in activation over the visual task in the bilateral superior temporal gyrus, corresponding to that in the primary auditory cortex, with an additional activation of the left greater than the right posterior insula. No significant posterior insular activation was seen during the visual task.

Differences between Populations in Auditory Task Activation

To compare autism and control populations, we performed a second-level, 2-sample t test design, and control > autism and autism > control contrasts were evaluated. Fig 3illustrates control > autism contrast, shown for an acceptable FDR of P < .05. Activity was seen for this contrast only in the left posterior insula, with peak activity at MNI coordinates x = −39, y = −28, z = 16, and a t-score of 5.7, corresponding to the FDR-corrected P value of .0087. No other areas of significantly differential activation were seen.

Fig 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 3.

Areas of greater activation for control than autism subjects for auditory language tasks. A, Control > autism activation for P < .05, FDR. Color bar shows t-scores. B, Blood oxygen level–dependent time series data for clusters are shown above for the entire auditory language task in autism and control populations, averaged across subjects for each group. Thin traces show standard error of the mean across subjects for each group.

Autism > control contrast demonstrated a cluster in the right posterior middle temporal gyrus, along the posterior inferior aspect of the Wernicke homolog at MNI coordinates x = 48, y = −55, z = 4, with a t-score = 4.1. To further evaluate this region, we performed a small volume correction by limiting evaluation to the right Wernicke homolog, given our a priori hypothesis that autistic subjects would show increased right-hemispheric activation of language regions. We limited evaluation by searching within a 2-cm diameter sphere that enclosed all significantly activated voxels in the right Wernicke homolog region on the group-activation map obtained from all subjects. With this correction, this cluster was significant at P = .004, FDR. No other foci of significantly different activation were seen for autism > control contrast.

Laterality Indices in Control and Autism Populations

To test for significant differences in language lateralization between autism and control samples, we identified receptive-language clusters in the left and right hemisphere for each subject. Receptive-language regions were selected because our task was designed primarily for receptive-language activation.

Left and right hemispheric clusters in the Wernicke region and homolog were identified for each subject after masking out the primary auditory cortex by using auditory > visual contrast. Laterality indices are shown in Fig 4, with population statistics listed in Table 1. There was significantly greater relative activation of right-sided language regions in the autism group, which was out of proportion to slight differences in hand preference as measured by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory, indicating that language lateralization is not merely a consequence of known population biases toward decreased right-handedness in autism. Moreover, these differences in laterality were observed in data for which variance associated with hand preference were already included as a regressor in the group-level analysis.

Fig 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 4.

Hand preference and language laterality for autism and control subjects. Histograms show the number of subjects exhibiting scores between −100 and 100 (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory) or between −1 and 1 (functional MR imaging [fMRI] laterality index), in which 100 represents strong right-handedness and 1 represents strong left-hemispheric language dominance.

Effect of Age, vIQ, and Standardized Language Scores on Activation

Four covariables were included in the regression when evaluating group-level activation: age, handedness, vIQ, and CELF-3 receptive-language function score. No significant foci of differential activation were associated with handedness. Increased proficiency on receptive-language testing (CELF-3) was associated with increased activation in the posterior left Wernicke area (Fig 5A, red), significant at P < .05, FDR. Increased vIQ scores were associated with a trend toward activation in the bilateral posterior inferior frontal gyrus (left Broca area and right hemispheric homolog), right putamen, left dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, left lateral premotor cortex, and right anterior temporal pole (Fig 5A, blue). These clusters were all significant at P < .001, uncorrected, but did not survive FDR correction at P = .05. MNI coordinates of activation associated with CELF-3 and vIQ tests are listed in Table 3. With younger age, activation was seen in the region of the bilateral nucleus accumbens, with peak activity at x = −3, y = −1, z = −5, t-score = 4.8, corresponding to an acceptable FDR P value of .031. This cluster is shown in Fig 5B. No significant differential activation was associated with older age, lower vIQ score, or lower CELF-3 score.

Fig 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 5.

Associations between auditory language activation and covariables for all subjects. A, Activity associated with higher receptive-language (Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 3rd ed) scores (red, P < .05, FDR) and higher verbal intelligence quotients (blue, P < .001, uncorrected). B, Activity associated with younger age (P < .05, FDR).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3:

MNI coordinates of increased activation associated with standardized test scores

Discussion

Decreased Posterior Insular Activity during Language in Autism

We found a significant decrease in activation in the posterior insula during auditory language processing in autism compared with typically developing participants. This area showed striking activation during the language task for controls, with a t-score second only to that in the primary auditory cortex, greater than scores for either the Wernicke area or Broca area on the left. The posterior insula was activated only during our auditory and not our visual language task; this finding suggests an auditory processing function to this region. This idea is supported by a recent study in which electrophysiologic recordings in rhesus monkeys (which also show a close proximity of the posterior insula to the Heschl gyrus) demonstrated selective responses in posterior insular neurons to sounds associated with vocal communication.35

A longitudinal study in children 5–11 years of age (younger than participants in our study) showed decreased left posterior insular activity with age in virtually identical coordinates to the focus we detected,36 a finding that may suggest an even more important role for this region in language development. The posterior insula has also been implicated in other features relevant to the autism phenotype, including emotive processing of stimuli such as experiencing pain,37 processing negative emotions such as disgust,38 and emotional responses to aversive facial stimuli.39

Language Hemispheric Lateralization in Autism

We found similar activation magnitude in the Broca area and in its right homolog in the autism and control groups. All autism studies measuring lateralization during language tasks by using the fMRI BOLD response or blood-flow velocity have found left lateralization in the activation of the Broca area compared with its right homolog in autism, though the degree of lateralization and activation in autism relative to controls has differed across studies.22,27,40,41

In contrast to frontal language regions, the pattern of activation in the Wernicke area and its right homolog differed in our autism and typically developing control samples. Although the amount of activation in the Wernicke area in the left hemisphere was similar in autism and control participants, activation in the right homolog was more extensive in autism, as measured both by population-level activity in the right homolog, though this difference was small, and by population statistics on the laterality index that showed a greater right-sided spatial extent of activation. This difference in language laterality does not merely reflect the known differences in hand preference in our sample, because a quantitative metric of hand preference showed a slight, nonsignificant trend toward decreased right-handedness, whereas the language laterality observed with the fMRI laterality index was significant and much more pronounced.

Differential Activation Associated with Age and Standardized Testing

We noted several associations with standardized testing and our fMRI results. First, participants with higher scores for receptive-language proficiency, as measured by CELF-3, showed increased spatial extent of the left Wernicke area, with recruitment of more of the posterior middle temporal gyrus. This seems plausible given the known dominant receptive-language function of the left Wernicke area. Second, participants with higher verbal IQ scores demonstrated greater recruitment of frontal language areas, including the left Broca area and its right-sided homolog, as well as the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and lateral premotor cortex. This finding is consistent with a more complex frontal language network in participants with higher IQs.

Finally, we saw activation of the bilateral nucleus accumbens in younger participants. This area has been associated with neural processing of rewards,42 including novel stimuli and natural reinforcement.43 It is possible that younger subjects experience a greater sense of intrinsic approval when “solving” the auditory-phrase task compared with older subjects.

Study Limitations

We included a higher total number of autism participants in the study to better account for the known clinical heterogeneity of autism but acknowledge that the observed findings may not be uniformly generalized to represent differing clinical autism phenotypes. We also elected not to have the subjects speak during the task to prevent head motion, which limited our ability to compare subject performance on the task between the groups. We observed very similar activation patterns, however, in most activated regions, suggesting that language network activation was similarly achieved in the 2 populations. Finally, the language stimuli used were short phrases that lack the context of normal conversation and may not generalize to conversational language.

Conclusions

High-functioning autistic subjects showed significantly decreased involvement of the left posterior insula during auditory language processing, a finding that may correlate with impaired perception of emotive content in language. We also found increased activation in the right hemispheric Wernicke homolog, consistent with prior reports of atypical functional lateralization in expressive language regions. These findings suggest targets for ongoing investigations of the neurophysiologic mechanism of language abnormalities in autism.

Acknowledgments

We thank Jim Lee, PhD, Melody Johnson, and Henry Buswell, of the University of Utah Center for Advanced Imaging Research, for technical assistance in data acquisition and paradigm design. We also thank Barbara Young and Celeste Knowles of the Utah Autism Neuroscience Program and express our sincere gratitude to the young people and their families who participated in the study.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by grant RO1 MH080826 (J.E.L., A.L.A., N.L., E.D.B.) from the National Institutes of Health (NIH), an Autism Speaks Predoctoral Fellowship Grant (M.B.D.), and a University of Utah Multidisciplinary Research seed grant (J.S.A., J.E.L.). Additional support came from National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) R01 NS34783 (NL), National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) P50 MH60450 (NL), and Ben B. and Iris M. Margolis Foundation (J.S.A.). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH, NINDS, or the NIH.

  • Paper previously presented in part at: Annual International Meeting for Autism Research, May 7–9, 2009; Chicago, Illinois.

Indicates open access to non-subscribers at www.ajnr.org

References

  1. 1.↵
    1. Kjelgaard MM,
    2. Tager-Flusberg H
    . An investigation of language impairment in autism: implications for genetic subgroups. Lang Cogn Process 2001; 16: 287–308
    CrossRefPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    1. Howlin P,
    2. Goode S,
    3. Hutton J,
    4. et al
    . Adult outcome for children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004; 45: 212–29
    CrossRefPubMed
  3. 3.↵
    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV-TR. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000
  4. 4.↵
    1. Raichle ME
    . Visualizing the mind. Sci Am 1994; 270: 58–64
    PubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Gomot M,
    2. Bernard FA,
    3. Davis MH,
    4. et al
    . Change detection in children with autism: an auditory event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 2006; 29: 475–84
    CrossRefPubMed
  6. 6.↵
    1. Nation K,
    2. Clarke P,
    3. Wright B,
    4. et al
    . Patterns of reading ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 2006; 36: 911–19
    CrossRefPubMed
  7. 7.↵
    1. Folstein SE,
    2. Santangelo SL,
    3. Gilman SE,
    4. et al
    . Predictors of cognitive test patterns in autism families. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40: 1117–28
    CrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Escalante-Mead PR,
    2. Minshew NJ,
    3. Sweeney JA
    . Abnormal brain lateralization in high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2003; 33: 539–43
    CrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.↵
    1. Coren S
    1. Bryson SE
    . Autism and anomalous handedness. In: Coren S, ed. Left Handedness. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier; 1990: 441–56
  10. 10.↵
    1. Colby KM,
    2. Parkison C
    . Handedness in autistic children. J Autism Child Schizophr 1977; 7: 3–9
    CrossRefPubMed
  11. 11.↵
    1. Prior MR,
    2. Bradshaw JL
    . Hemisphere functioning in autistic children. Cortex 1979; 15: 73–81
    PubMed
  12. 12.↵
    1. Chiron C,
    2. Leboyer M,
    3. Leon F,
    4. et al
    . SPECT of the brain in childhood autism: evidence for a lack of normal hemispheric asymmetry. Dev Med Child Neurol 1995; 37: 849–60
    PubMed
  13. 13.↵
    1. Muller RA,
    2. Behen ME,
    3. Rothermel RD,
    4. et al
    . Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: a PET study. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29: 19–31
    CrossRefPubMed
  14. 14.↵
    1. Stroganova TA,
    2. Nygren G,
    3. Tsetlin MM,
    4. et al
    . Abnormal EEG lateralization in boys with autism. Clin Neurophysiol 2007; 118: 1842–54. Epub 2007 Jun 19
    CrossRefPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    1. Flagg EJ,
    2. Cardy JE,
    3. Roberts W,
    4. et al
    . Language lateralization development in children with autism: insights from the late field magnetoencephalogram. Neurosci Lett 2005; 386: 82–87
    CrossRefPubMed
  16. 16.↵
    1. De Fosse L,
    2. Hodge SM,
    3. Makris N,
    4. et al
    . Language-association cortex asymmetry in autism and specific language impairment. Ann Neurol 2004; 56: 757–66
    CrossRefPubMed
  17. 17.↵
    1. Herbert MR,
    2. Ziegler DA,
    3. Deutsch CK,
    4. et al
    . Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested whole-brain analysis. Brain 2005; 128(Pt 1): 213–26. Epub 2004 Nov 24
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  18. 18.↵
    1. Bigler ED,
    2. Mortensen S,
    3. Neeley ES,
    4. et al
    . Superior temporal gyrus, language function, and autism. Dev Neuropsychol 2007; 31: 217–38
    PubMed
  19. 19.↵
    1. Dawson G,
    2. Finley C,
    3. Phillips S,
    4. et al
    . Hemispheric specialization and the language abilities of autistic children. Child Dev 1986; 57: 1440–53
    CrossRefPubMed
  20. 20.↵
    1. Boddaert N,
    2. Chabane N,
    3. Gervais H,
    4. et al
    . Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: a voxel-based morphometry MRI study. Neuroimage 2004; 23: 364–69
    CrossRefPubMed
  21. 21.↵
    1. Wilson TW,
    2. Rojas DC,
    3. Reite ML,
    4. et al
    . Children and adolescents with autism exhibit reduced MEG steady-state gamma responses. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62: 192–97. Epub 2006 Sep 6
    CrossRefPubMed
  22. 22.↵
    1. Kleinhans NM,
    2. Muller RA,
    3. Cohen DN,
    4. et al
    . Atypical functional lateralization of language in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res 2008; 1221: 115–25
    CrossRefPubMed
  23. 23.↵
    1. Bruneau N,
    2. Bonnet-Brilhault F,
    3. Gomot M,
    4. et al
    . Cortical auditory processing and communication in children with autism: electrophysiological/behavioral relations. Int J Psychophysiol 2003; 51: 17–25
    CrossRefPubMed
  24. 24.↵
    1. Bruneau N,
    2. Roux S,
    3. Adrien JL,
    4. et al
    . Auditory associative cortex dysfunction in children with autism: evidence from late auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave-T complex). Clin Neurophysiol 1999; 110: 1927–34
    CrossRefPubMed
  25. 25.↵
    1. Dawson G,
    2. Finley C,
    3. Phillips S,
    4. et al
    . A comparison of hemispheric asymmetries in speech-related brain potentials of autistic and dysphasic children. Brain Lang 1989; 37: 26–41
    CrossRefPubMed
  26. 26.↵
    1. Boddaert N,
    2. Belin P,
    3. Chabane N,
    4. et al
    . Perception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 2057–60
    CrossRefPubMed
  27. 27.↵
    1. Knaus TA,
    2. Silver AM,
    3. Lindgren KA,
    4. et al
    . fMRI activation during a language task in adolescents with ASD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008; 14: 967–79
    CrossRefPubMed
  28. 28.↵
    1. Lord C,
    2. Rutter M,
    3. Le Couteur A
    . Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24: 659–85
    CrossRefPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Lord C,
    2. Risi S,
    3. Lambrecht L,
    4. et al
    . The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30: 205–23
    CrossRefPubMed
  30. 30.↵
    1. Leyfer OT,
    2. Folstein SE,
    3. Bacalman S,
    4. et al
    . Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: interview development and rates of disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 2006; 36: 849–61
    CrossRefPubMed
  31. 31.↵
    1. Oldfield RC
    . The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971; 9: 97–113
    CrossRefPubMed
  32. 32.↵
    1. Semel E,
    2. Wiig EH,
    3. Secord WA
    . Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals. 3rd ed. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1995
  33. 33.↵
    1. Gaillard WD,
    2. Balsamo L,
    3. Xu B,
    4. et al
    . Language dominance in partial epilepsy patients identified with an fMRI reading task. Neurology 2002; 59: 256–65
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  34. 34.↵
    1. Xu J,
    2. Kemeny S,
    3. Park G,
    4. et al
    . Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension. Neuroimage 2005; 25: 1002–15
    CrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Remedios R,
    2. Logothetis NK,
    3. Kayser C
    . An auditory region in the primate insular cortex responding preferentially to vocal communication sounds. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 1034–45
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  36. 36.↵
    1. Szaflarski JP,
    2. Schmithorst VJ,
    3. Altaye M,
    4. et al
    . A longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study of language development in children 5 to 11 years old. Ann Neurol 2006; 59: 796–807
    CrossRefPubMed
  37. 37.↵
    1. Singer T,
    2. Seymour B,
    3. O'Doherty J,
    4. et al
    . Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 2004; 303: 1157–62
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  38. 38.↵
    1. Britton JC,
    2. Phan KL,
    3. Taylor SF,
    4. et al
    . Neural correlates of social and nonsocial emotions: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2006; 31: 397–409
    CrossRefPubMed
  39. 39.↵
    1. Anderson IM,
    2. Del-Ben CM,
    3. McKie S,
    4. et al
    . Citalopram modulation of neuronal responses to aversive face emotions: a functional MRI study. Neuroreport 2007; 18: 1351–55
    CrossRefPubMed
  40. 40.↵
    1. Just MA,
    2. Cherkassky VL,
    3. Keller TA,
    4. et al
    . Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain 2004; 127: 1811–21
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  41. 41.↵
    1. Tesink CM,
    2. Buitelaar JK,
    3. Petersson KM,
    4. et al
    . Neural correlates of pragmatic language comprehension in autism spectrum disorders. Brain 2009; 132(Pt 7): 1941–52. Epub 2009 May 7
    Abstract/FREE Full Text
  42. 42.↵
    1. Ikemoto S,
    2. Panksepp J
    . The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1999; 31: 6–41
    CrossRefPubMed
  43. 43.↵
    1. Salamone JD,
    2. Correa M,
    3. Mingote SM,
    4. et al
    . Beyond the reward hypothesis: alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2005; 5: 34–41
    CrossRefPubMed
  • Received May 28, 2009.
  • Accepted after revision June 15, 2009.
  • Copyright © American Society of Neuroradiology
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

American Journal of Neuroradiology: 31 (1)
American Journal of Neuroradiology
Vol. 31, Issue 1
1 Jan 2010
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Advertisement
Print
Download PDF
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Journal of Neuroradiology.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Decreased Left Posterior Insular Activity during Auditory Language in Autism
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Journal of Neuroradiology
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Journal of Neuroradiology web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Cite this article
J.S. Anderson, N. Lange, A. Froehlich, M.B. DuBray, T.J. Druzgal, M.P. Froimowitz, A.L. Alexander, E.D. Bigler, J.E. Lainhart
Decreased Left Posterior Insular Activity during Auditory Language in Autism
American Journal of Neuroradiology Jan 2010, 31 (1) 131-139; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A1789

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
0 Responses
Respond to this article
Share
Bookmark this article
Decreased Left Posterior Insular Activity during Auditory Language in Autism
J.S. Anderson, N. Lange, A. Froehlich, M.B. DuBray, T.J. Druzgal, M.P. Froimowitz, A.L. Alexander, E.D. Bigler, J.E. Lainhart
American Journal of Neuroradiology Jan 2010, 31 (1) 131-139; DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A1789
del.icio.us logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
Purchase

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusions
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Responses
  • References
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Parsing Brain Network Specialization: A Replication and Expansion of Wang et al. (2014)
  • Reduced Lateralization of Multiple Functional Brain Networks in Autistic Males
  • Maladaptive Laterality in Cortical Networks Related to Social Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorder
  • Maladaptive laterality in cortical networks related to social communication in autism spectrum disorder
  • Reduced language lateralization in autism and the broader autism phenotype as assessed with robust individual-subjects analyses
  • The small and efficient language network of polyglots and hyper-polyglots
  • Crossref (66)
  • Google Scholar

This article has been cited by the following articles in journals that are participating in Crossref Cited-by Linking.

  • Decreased Interhemispheric Functional Connectivity in Autism
    J. S. Anderson, T. J. Druzgal, A. Froehlich, M. B. DuBray, N. Lange, A. L. Alexander, T. Abildskov, J. A. Nielsen, A. N. Cariello, J. R. Cooperrider, E. D. Bigler, J. E. Lainhart
    Cerebral Cortex 2011 21 5
  • Auditory processing in autism spectrum disorder: A review
    K. O’Connor
    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2012 36 2
  • A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism
    Lisa T. Eyler, Karen Pierce, Eric Courchesne
    Brain 2012 135 3
  • Network anticorrelations, global regression, and phase‐shifted soft tissue correction
    Jeffrey S. Anderson, T. Jason Druzgal, Melissa Lopez‐Larson, Eun‐Kee Jeong, Krishnaji Desai, Deborah Yurgelun‐Todd
    Human Brain Mapping 2011 32 6
  • The role of the insula in speech and language processing
    Anna Oh, Emma G. Duerden, Elizabeth W. Pang
    Brain and Language 2014 135
  • An Evaluation of the Left-Brain vs. Right-Brain Hypothesis with Resting State Functional Connectivity Magnetic Resonance Imaging
    Jared A. Nielsen, Brandon A. Zielinski, Michael A. Ferguson, Janet E. Lainhart, Jeffrey S. Anderson, Yong He
    PLoS ONE 2013 8 8
  • Atypicalities in Cortical Structure, Handedness, and Functional Lateralization for Language in Autism Spectrum Disorders
    Annukka K. Lindell, Kristelle Hudry
    Neuropsychology Review 2013 23 3
  • Functional magnetic resonance imaging of autism spectrum disorders
    Gabriel S. Dichter
    Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 2012 14 3
  • Abnormal lateralization of functional connectivity between language and default mode regions in autism
    Jared A Nielsen, Brandon A Zielinski, P Thomas Fletcher, Andrew L Alexander, Nicholas Lange, Erin D Bigler, Janet E Lainhart, Jeffrey S Anderson
    Molecular Autism 2014 5 1
  • Atypically rightward cerebral asymmetry in male adults with autism stratifies individuals with and without language delay
    Dorothea L. Floris, Meng‐Chuan Lai, Tibor Auer, Michael V. Lombardo, Christine Ecker, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Sally J. Wheelwright, Edward T. Bullmore, Declan G.M. Murphy, Simon Baron‐Cohen, John Suckling
    Human Brain Mapping 2016 37 1

More in this TOC Section

  • Glutaric Aciduria Type 1: DK vs. Conventional MRI
  • Kurtosis and Epileptogenic Tubers: A Pilot Study
  • Brain Iron in Niemann-Pick Type C: 7T Study
Show more Functional

Similar Articles

Advertisement

Indexed Content

  • Current Issue
  • Accepted Manuscripts
  • Article Preview
  • Past Issues
  • Editorials
  • Editors Choice
  • Fellow Journal Club
  • Letters to the Editor

Cases

  • Case Collection
  • Archive - Case of the Week
  • Archive - Case of the Month
  • Archive - Classic Case

Special Collections

  • Special Collections

Resources

  • News and Updates
  • Turn around Times
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Author Policies
  • Manuscript Submission Guidelines
  • Evidence-Based Medicine Level Guide
  • Publishing Checklists
  • Graphical Abstract Preparation
  • Imaging Protocol Submission
  • Submit a Case
  • Become a Reviewer/Academy of Reviewers
  • Get Peer Review Credit from Publons

Multimedia

  • AJNR Podcast
  • AJNR SCANtastic
  • Video Articles

About Us

  • About AJNR
  • Editorial Board
  • Not an AJNR Subscriber? Join Now
  • Alerts
  • Feedback
  • Advertise with us
  • Librarian Resources
  • Permissions
  • Terms and Conditions

American Society of Neuroradiology

  • Not an ASNR Member? Join Now

© 2025 by the American Society of Neuroradiology All rights, including for text and data mining, AI training, and similar technologies, are reserved.
Print ISSN: 0195-6108 Online ISSN: 1936-959X

Powered by HighWire